A flood of
new research is overturning old assumptions about what animal minds are and
aren’t capable of – and changing how we think about our own species
----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Giraffes
will eat courgettes if they have to, but they really prefer carrots. A team of
researchers from Spain and Germany recently took advantage of this preference
to investigate whether the animals are capable of statistical reasoning. In the
experiment, a giraffe was shown two transparent containers holding a mixture of
carrot and courgette slices. One container held mostly carrots, the other
mostly courgettes. A researcher then took one slice from each container and
offered them to the giraffe with closed hands, so it couldn’t see which
vegetable had been selected.
In repeated
trials, the four test giraffes reliably chose the hand that had reached into
the container with more carrots, showing they understood that the more carrots
were in the container, the more likely it was that a carrot had been picked.
Monkeys have passed similar tests, and human babies can do it at 12 months old.
But giraffes’ brains are much smaller than primates’ relative to body size, so
it was notable to see how well they grasped the concept.
Such
discoveries are becoming less surprising every year, however, as a flood of new
research overturns longstanding assumptions about what animal minds are and
aren’t capable of. A recent wave of popular books on animal cognition argue
that skills long assumed to be humanity’s prerogative, from planning for the
future to a sense of fairness, actually exist throughout the animal kingdom –
and not just in primates or other mammals, but in birds, octopuses and beyond.
In 2018, for instance, a team at the University of Buenos Aires found evidence
that zebra finches, whose brains weigh half a gram, have dreams. Monitors
attached to the birds’ throats found that when they were asleep, their muscles
sometimes moved in exactly the same pattern as when they were singing out loud;
in other words, they seemed to be dreaming about singing.
In the 21st
century, findings such as these are helping to drive a major shift in the way
human beings think about animals – and about ourselves. Humanity has
traditionally justified its supremacy over all other animals – the fact that we
breed them and keep them in cages, rather than vice versa – by our intellectual
superiority. According to Aristotle, humans are distinguished from other living
things because only we possess a rational soul. We know our species as Homo
sapiens, “wise man”.
Yet at a
time when humanity’s self-image is largely shaped by fears of environmental
devastation and nuclear war, combined with memories of historical atrocity, it
is no longer so easy to say, with Hamlet, that man is “the paragon of animals”
– the ideal that other creatures would imitate, if only they could. Nature may
be “red in tooth and claw”, but creatures whose weapons are teeth and claws can
only kill each other one at a time. Only humans commit atrocities such as war,
genocide and slavery – and what allows us to conceive and carry out such crimes
is the very power of reason that we boast about.
In his 2022
book If Nietzsche Were a Narwhal, Justin Gregg, a specialist in dolphin
communication, takes this mistrust of human reason to an extreme. The book’s
title encapsulates Gregg’s argument: if Friedrich Nietzsche had been born a
narwhal instead of a German philosopher, he would have been much better off,
and given his intellectual influence on fascism, so would the world. By
extension, the same is true of our whole species. “The planet does not love us
as much as we love our intellect,” Gregg writes. “We have generated more death
and destruction for life on this planet than any other animal, past and
present. Our many intellectual accomplishments are currently on track to
produce our own extinction.”
If human
minds are incapable of solving the problems they create, then perhaps our
salvation lies in encountering very different types of minds. The global
popularity of the documentary My Octopus Teacher, released by Netflix in 2020,
is just one example of the growing hunger for such encounters. In the film, the
South African diver Craig Foster spends months filming a female octopus in an
underwater kelp forest, observing most of her lifecycle. Foster presents
himself as the anti-Jacques Cousteau; he doesn’t go underwater to study the
non-human, but to learn from it.
Humility is
a traditional religious discipline, and there is a spiritual dimension to
Foster’s quest and to the film’s success. On YouTube, where the trailer has
been viewed 3.7m times, thousands of people testify that My Octopus Teacher
made them weep, changed their understanding of the world and made them resolve
to lead better lives. It’s clear that, for modern people who seldom encounter
animals except for pet cats and dogs, entering into a close relationship with a
non-human mind can be a sacred experience.
The idea of
the octopus as the nonhuman mind par excellence was popularised by the 2016
bestseller Other Minds: The Octopus, the Sea, and the Deep Origins of
Consciousness, by Peter Godfrey-Smith. A philosopher rather than a marine
biologist, Godfrey-Smith got an opportunity to see the creatures in action at a
site off eastern Australia known to researchers as Octopolis. There he
discovered that octopuses are “smart in the sense of being curious and
flexible; they are adventurous, opportunistic”, prone to making off with items
such as tape measures and measuring stakes.
The
fascination of the octopus is that while its behaviour seems recognisable in
human terms as mischief or curiosity, its neural architecture is immensely
different from ours. Since Darwin, humans have grown used to recognising
ourselves in our fellow primates, whose brains and body plans are similar to
our own. After all, humans and chimpanzees share a common ape ancestor that
lived in Africa as recently as 6m years ago. Our most recent common ancestor
with the octopus, by contrast, is a worm-like creature thought to have lived 500-600m
years ago.
Because the
mind of the octopus evolved in a completely different fashion from ours, it
makes sense of the world in ways we can barely imagine. An octopus has 500m
neurons, about as many as a dog, but most of these neurons are located not in
the brain but in its eight arms, each of which can move, smell and perhaps even
remember on its own. In Godfrey-Smith’s words, an octopus is “probably the
closest we will come to meeting an intelligent alien”. When such a being
encounters a human at the bottom of the ocean, what could it possibly make of
us?
For most of
the 20th century, animal researchers wouldn’t even have asked such a question,
much less attempted to answer it. Under the influence of the American
psychologist BF Skinner, scientific orthodoxy held that it was neither
legitimate nor necessary to talk about what was going on in an animal’s mind.
Science, he argued, only deals with things that can be observed and measured,
and we can’t directly observe mental faculties even in ourselves, much less in
animals. What we can observe is action and behaviour, and Skinner was able to
modify the behaviour of rats using positive reinforcement, such as rewards of
food, and negative reinforcement, such as electric shocks.
Today, the
pendulum has swung in the other direction. Scientists speak without
embarrassment about animal minds and consciousness. In popular writing on the
subject, Skinner appears only as a villain. In his 2016 book Are We Smart
Enough to Know How Smart Animals Are?, primatologist Frans de Waal discusses a
mid-20th-century experiment in which researchers at a primate centre in
Florida, educated in Skinner’s methods, tried to train chimps the way he had
trained rats, by withholding food. “Expressing no interest in cognition – the
existence of which they didn’t even acknowledge,” De Waal writes, the
researchers “investigated reinforcement schedules and the punitive effect of
time-outs.” The staff of the primate centre rebelled and started feeding the
chimps in secret, causing Skinner to lament that “tender-hearted colleagues
frustrated efforts to reduce chimpanzees to a satisfactory state of
deprivation”. You could hardly ask for a better example of how the arrogance of
reason leads to cruelty.
Meanwhile,
animals without “rational souls” are capable of demonstrating admirable
qualities such as patience and self-restraint. Among humans, the ability to
sacrifice immediate pleasure for future gain is called resisting temptation,
and is taken as a sign of maturity. But De Waal shows that even birds are
capable of it. In one experiment, an African grey parrot named Griffin was
taught that if he resisted the urge to eat a serving of cereal, he would be
rewarded after an unpredictable interval with food he liked better, such as
cashew nuts. The bird was able to hold out 90% of the time, devising ways to
distract himself by talking, preening his feathers, or simply throwing the cup
of cereal across the room. Such behaviours, De Waal notes, are quite similar to
what human children do in the face of temptation.
More
intriguing than the convergences between human and animal behaviour, however,
are the profound differences in the way we perceive and experience the world.
The reason why an encounter with an octopus can be awe-inspiring is that two
species endowed with different senses and brains inhabit the same planet but
very different realities.
Take the
sense of smell. As humans, we learn about our surroundings primarily by seeing
and hearing, while our ability to detect odours is fairly undeveloped. For many
animals, the reverse is true. In his 2022 book An Immense World, the science
journalist Ed Yong writes about an experiment by researcher Lucy Bates
involving African elephants. Bates found that if she took urine from an
elephant in the rear of a herd and spread it on the ground in front of the
herd, the elephants reacted with bewilderment and curiosity, knowing that the
individual’s distinctive odour was coming from the wrong place. For them, a
smell out of place was as fundamental a violation of reality as a ghostly
apparition would be for us.
If giraffes
can do statistical reasoning and parrots understand the concept of the future,
then where does the distinctiveness of the human mind really lie? One favourite
candidate is what psychologists call “theory of mind” – the ability to infer
that each person is their own “I”, with independent experiences and private
mental states. In The Book of Minds, the science writer Philip Ball describes
the classic experiment that tests the development of this ability in children.
A child and an adult watch as an object is hidden under one of three cups. Then
the adult leaves the room and the child sees a second adult come in and move
the object so it’s under a different cup.
When the
first adult returns, where does the child expect she will look for the object?
Very young children assume that she will know its new location, just as they
do. Starting around age four, however, children start to understand that the
adult only knows what she has seen herself, so they expect her to look under
the original, now empty cup. “Indeed,” Ball writes, “they will often delight in
the deception: in their knowing what others don’t.”
Developing
a theory of mind is necessary because we can never know what is going on inside
other people in the same immediate way we know ourselves. Most adults take for
granted that other people have the same kind of inner life they do, but this
remains a kind of assumption. René Descartes was one of the first philosophers
to wrestle with this problem, in the 17th century. “What do I see from the
window but hats and coats which may cover automatic machines?” he asked. “Yet I
judge these to be men.” But Descartes didn’t extend the same benefit of the
doubt to animals. Even more than Skinner, he saw them as automata without any
inner experience, “bêtes-machines”. Ball notes that Descartes dissected live
animals to study the circulation of the blood, “and dismissed any cries of pain
that procedure elicited as a mere mechanical response, not unlike the screech
of a poorly oiled axle”.
Four
centuries later, De Waal complains that science still hasn’t overcome the
tendency to draw a dividing line between the inner lives of humans and those of
other creatures. The reason that scientists have focused on theory of mind, De
Waal believes, is because no animal has been shown to possess it. Such
“interspecific bragging contests”, he writes, are designed to flatter our sense
of superiority. In fact, it seems that even here we’re not clear winners.
According to Ball, recent attempts to replicate the theory-of-mind experiment
with chimps and bonobos suggest that the majority of them pass the test, though
the evidence is ambiguous: since the subjects can’t talk, researchers gauge
their expectations by tracking their eye movements.
But are we
right to think of intelligence as a ladder in the first place? Maybe we should
think, instead, in terms of what Ball calls “the space of possible minds” – the
countless potential ways of understanding the world, some of which we may not
even be able to imagine. In mapping this space, which could theoretically
include computer and extraterrestrial minds as well as animal ones, “we are
currently no better placed than the pre-Copernican astronomers who installed
the Earth at the centre of the cosmos and arranged everything else in relation
to it”, Ball observes. Until we know more about what kinds of minds are
possible, it is sheer hubris to set up our own as the standard of excellence.
Xenophanes,
a pre-Socratic philosopher, observed that if horses and oxen could draw
pictures, they would make the gods look like horses and oxen. Similarly, if
non-human beings could devise a test of intelligence, they might rank species
according to, say, their ability to find their way home from a distance
unaided. Bees do this by detecting magnetic fields, and dogs by following
odours, while most modern humans would be helpless without a map or a GPS.
“Earth is bursting with animal species that have hit on solutions for how to
live a good life in ways that put the human species to shame,” Gregg says.
But if
human and animal minds are so essentially different that we can never truly
understand one another, then a troubling thought arises: we would be less like
neighbours than inmates who occupy separate cells in the same prison. The kind
of understanding Foster achieved with his octopus, or Goodall with her
chimpanzees, would have to be written off as an anthropomorphising illusion,
just as Skinner warned.
The
possibility of true interspecies understanding is the subject of Thomas Nagel’s
landmark 1974 essay What Is It Like to Be a Bat?, to which every writer on
animal cognition pays their respects, sometimes wearily. Nagel, an American
philosopher, concluded that humans can never really understand a bat’s inner
experience. Even if I try to picture what it’s like to fly on webbed wings and
spend most of my time hanging upside down, all I can imagine is what it would
be like for me to be a bat, not what it’s like for a bat to be a bat.
For Nagel,
this conclusion has implications beyond animal psychology. It proves that
mental life can never be reduced to things we can observe from the outside,
whether that means the way we behave or the pattern of electrical impulses in
our neurons. Subjectivity, what it feels like to exist, is so profoundly
different from what we can observe scientifically that the two realms can’t
even be described in the same language.
Few people
have ever taken the challenge of Nagel’s essay as literally as Charles Foster
in his 2016 book Being a Beast. A barrister and academic by profession, Foster
set himself the challenge of entering the mental worlds of five animal species
by living as much like them as possible. To be a fox, he writes: “I lay in a
back yard in Bow, foodless and drinkless, urinating and defecating where I was,
waiting for the night and treating as hostile the humans in the row houses all
around.” To be a badger, he dug a trench in the side of a hill and lived inside
it with his young son Tom, eating earthworms and inhaling dust. “Tom was
filling tissues with silica and blood for a week,” Foster notes.
Foster
welcomes all this damage and discomfort, but not in the spirit of a scientist
doing fieldwork. Rather, he evokes the medieval flagellants who covered their
backs with welts to purge themselves of sin. That Foster defines sin as a
transgression against nature rather than God doesn’t make the concept any less
religious. “Evolutionary biology is a numinous statement of the
interconnectedness of things,” he writes, and his preaching translates easily
into Christian terms: “Say, with Saint Francis, ‘Hello, Brother Ox,’ and mean
it,” he demands.
Foster’s
way of seeking communion with the animals may be extreme, at times comically
so, but his basic impulse is shared by many of today’s students of animal
cognition, and an increasing number of laypeople as well. Encountering an
animal mind can perform the same function as a great work of art or a religious
experience: it makes the familiar strange, reminding us that reality
encompasses far more than we ordinarily think.
The great
difference is that while a traditional religious experience can awaken human
beings to God, an animal epiphany can awakens us to the fullness of this world.
“What she taught me was to feel that you’re part of this place, not a visitor,”
Foster says in the closing lines of My Octopus Teacher, and by “this place” he
doesn’t just mean a particular kelp forest, but the Earth itself. At first this
might sound like an odd realisation: where else would human beings belong if
not on our one and only planet?
But in the
21st century, it is clearly becoming harder for us to think of ourselves as
genuinely belonging to the Earth. Whether we look back on our long history of
driving other species to extinction, or forward to a future in which we
extinguish ourselves through climate breakdown, many humans now see humanity as
the greatest danger facing the Earth – a cancer that grows without limit,
killing its host.
It is no
coincidence that, at the same moment, tech visionaries have begun to think
about our future in extraterrestrial terms. Earth may be where humanity
happened to evolve, they say, but our destiny calls us to other worlds. Elon
Musk founded SpaceX in 2002 with the explicit goal of hastening humanity’s
colonisation of Mars. Other “transhumanist” thinkers look forward to a fully
virtual future, in which our minds leave our bodies behind and achieve
immortality in the form of electromagnetic pulses.
These
projects sound futuristic, but they are best understood as new expressions of a
very old human anxiety. We have always suffered from metaphysical claustrophobia
– the sense that a cosmos containing no minds but our own was intolerably
narrow. That is why, since prehistoric times, humans have populated Earth with
other kinds of intelligences – from gods and angels to fairies, forest-spirits
and demons. All premodern cultures took the existence of such non-human minds
for granted. In medieval Europe, Christian and Greek philosophical ideas gave
rise to the doctrine of the “great chain of being”, which held that the
universe is populated by an unbroken series of creatures, all the way from
plants at the bottom to God at the apex. Humanity stood in the middle, more
intelligent than the animals but less than the angels, who came in many
species, with different powers and purviews.
Our rising
interest in animal minds can be seen as a way of filling in the regions “below”
us as well. If an octopus is like an intelligent alien, as Godfrey-Smith
writes, then we don’t need to scan the skies so anxiously for an actual
extraterrestrial. Yong quotes Elizabeth Jakob, an American spider expert, to
the same effect: “We don’t have to look to aliens from other planets … We have
animals that have a completely different interpretation of what the world is
right next to us.” Perhaps simply knowing that these other minds exist can help
us make peace with the limitations of our own.
Can humans
ever understand how animals think? By Adam Kirsch. The Guardian, May 30, 2023.
Naturalist Charles Foster wanted to reconnect with his inner beast – so he tried living as a badger, a deer and a fox
Charles Foster lifts a wriggling worm to his lips. “Cheers,” he says.
We clink worms.
He warns that it might try to escape, and that there will be a strange, stereo-squirmy sensation when the worm splits in two.
He grimaces and chews pedantically to make sure every last bit has gone. Foster’s right: there is a grittiness to them. Earthworms wouldn’t be my first choice on the à la carte.
Foster has been on all fours for much of the morning, reminiscing about the periods he spent living as a badger, an otter, an urban fox. He also spent time, less successfully, as a red deer and a swift. In one way or another, he says, he has spent most of his 53 years being an animal, or at least trying to.
As a toddler in Manchester, he would look out of his bedroom window, fascinated by the tawny owl perched on a lamp outside. The family moved to Sheffield, where it was the blackbird in the garden he couldn’t stop thinking about. The bird stared at him with its yellow eye-rings and black pupils, taunting him. It knew something, and Foster wanted to know what.
“I was tantalised and infuriated,” he says. “I went down to the local library and read everything I could about blackbirds. I got a blackbird brain and pickled it in formalin, and would sleep holding it, wondering whether its wisdom would diffuse into me. I stuffed a blackbird and it circled on a piece of thread above my head when I went to sleep at night. I mapped every blackbird nest in our area and put them down in charts. I made maps of their flight path, and took ladders and looked into their nests. And I failed utterly to work out what made a blackbird tick.”
Foster was an unusual boy who grew into an unusual man. He was born to high-achieving, working-class parents who both became head teachers of primary schools. He had even greater ambitions for himself. His parents sent him to the local comprehensive, but he went to the library and read up about public school scholarships. Without telling his parents, he applied for one and won it. He left the comp to go to public schools far away in Bristol and Shrewsbury, where he entered a new world of privilege and expectation. Part of him hated it, but he thrived.
He promised himself he would re-engage with the natural world after his O-levels. “As soon as the last paper was finished, I went home and spent a day and a night lying in the Mayfield Valley just above our home in Sheffield, at a hare’s eye level. This was a sort of redemptive process that would undo all the life-denying process of the exams. That was my first conscious attempt to live like an animal.”
Did he enjoy it? “Enjoy isn’t the word. But I could feel a lot of toxins were being washed out. That seeded in me something – that in living the life we normally live, we are living a life that isn’t natural. And in order to be properly human, we’ve got to be properly animal.” Foster pauses and looks at me. “Do you think I’m insane?”
Convention got the better of him again. Foster sacrificed his inner hare and went to Cambridge University, where he studied veterinary science and law. After he left, he worked as a vet and a barrister, as an academic teaching medical law and ethics, as a philosopher and as a magazine columnist. (He still dabbles in all of these, and currently teaches at Oxford University.) He couldn’t have moved further away from the little boy who worshipped the spontaneity of nature. Instead of empathising with wild animals, Foster started shooting them for pleasure. He ran ultramarathons, 150 miles in a week, over the deserts of Africa. He made good money as a barrister, hunted in tweeds and lived the good life.
He soon felt a fraud. “The secret of a good barrister is that they are an empty vessel into which the soul of the latest murderer or rapist can be poured. It’s a very spiritually debilitating job to do. The damage that did called me to consider: what on earth is this creature called Charles Foster who occasionally voices opinions, who says he is a moral agent, who says he has ethical positions?”
Who did he think the real Charles Foster was? “The real Charles Foster grew up in a little semi in Sheffield and had his mates up and down the road, whereas I had remade myself as something else.” He saw himself as the kind of swashbuckling hero that Rider Haggard might have created. “I was a very arrogant, presumptuous, fantastically self-confident barrister. Even by barristers’ standards.”
One day he had an epiphany – well, an anti-epiphany. He was on an expedition across the Sinai, sitting in the midday sun, and he started to weigh up his achievements. “I thought: I will spend my time looking at the enormously fascinating, kaleidoscopically colourful soul of the fantastic Charles Foster. And I couldn’t see anything at all. It was like looking into a well. There was a rising sense of panic, from which it took years to recover.”
How did the fallout express itself? “A lot of the pinstriped swaggering vanished overnight,” he says. “The pride haemorrhaged away. I was a pretty broken person. I mean, I continued at the bar, I carried on working. But I became increasingly introspective.”
In his mid-30s, Foster’s first marriage fell apart and he started spending more time trying to understand what made animals tick: what is it like to be an otter guided by the smell of shit? Was he as competent a city dweller as an urban fox?
We stop at a spot in the woods. See, this could make a nice little sett, he says. He points to a small hollow covered with branches. Foster is a big man – 6ft 3in and a good 15 stone – but he eases himself into the hollow with surprising dexterity. When he lived as a badger in Wales with his son, Tom, they would sleep by day and prowl by night. In Being A Beast, the book he has written about his experiences, Foster wonderfully conveys the sensuality of life where smell is everything, and sight virtually irrelevant. But the book is very funny about his many bestial failings. Yes, he and Tom can cope in the summer, when badger life is a form of extreme camping, but returning in midwinter is a different matter. As an otter, he had less success. Part of the problem was he didn’t like otters: nasty killers with little to recommend them. He enjoyed the sprainting (shit-sniffing, which he did with his children; before long, they could distinguish each other’s poo) and spent hours swimming the lakes on Exmoor. But no, otter life didn’t do much to enrich the soul.
As for being a red deer, he really struggled. Yes, he grew his toenails and hair long, but these felt like superficial gestures. He asked a friend to set his bloodhound on him, so he could know what it was like to be hunted. There was an initial surge of adrenaline as he ran, but ultimately the experience was humiliating: when the dog tracked him down, it just gave him a contemptuous glance and walked off. Foster admits he was a rubbish deer. “I found it impossible to come down the pyramid and become a victim.”
By contrast, there is something ecstatic in his hopeless attempts to be a swift. He adores these most ethereal of birds, and is in awe of their ability to find their way from the eaves of his study in Oxford to their ancestral home in Congo and back again, to fly 30,000 miles a year without perching (swifts sleep and mate in the air). Foster goes paragliding, follows their path to Africa, eats similar foods; but it serves only to remind him how lumpen he is.
It is freezing but gloriously sunny in the woods. Foster slips out of his sett, takes off the mask the Guardian has lent him for the photoshoot, lights his tiny Hexamine cooker and knocks up a few barbecued worms, which he serves with nettles and garlic. They taste better cooked: less slippery.
You know, I say, I think you were most at home as a badger. “Why do you say that?” he asks. He looks disappointed. “I think I got closer to foxes than anyone else.”
I feel bad. Of course he made a much better fox: he got to scavenge through rubbish bins, mooch around London, share their emotional intelligence. “I wasn’t able to be olfactory enough as a badger,” Foster explains. “Another reason I didn’t get so close to badgers is that they are much more naturally relational animals than I am. I’m actually quite a lonely, wretched animal – and badgers aren’t.”
Tom made a great badger, I say. He puffs up with pride. “Yes, Tom’s a fantastic badger.”
We head off home where Mary, Foster’s second wife, has made pumpkin soup for lunch. She is a lovely, grounded woman who runs the house and looks after their four young children (Foster has two more from his first marriage); Mary also works as a GP one day a week. You can’t move in the house without bumping into stuffed birds, badgers, foxes and otters. A zebra skin dominates one wall in the lounge. I thought Foster despised his shooting days? “But it’s so beautiful.” Isn’t there something masochistic in having it there? “Yes,” he says, “it’s a bit like Christian ascetics constantly reminding themselves of the sins of the past.”
We sit down to lunch. I ask Mary if it took him long to adapt whenever he came back to domestic life. “He re-engaged with the family quite quickly,” she says. “The hardest thing for him was probably using knives and forks. Sitting at a table. Basic social mores. He likes standing up to eat anyway, walking around and doing things.”
Could she tell the difference when he returned after being a badger from, say, when he returned from being an otter? “I don’t think I could, no,” Mary answers gently. “I’m usually preoccupied with children’s bottoms and putting food on the table.” She looks at Foster affectionately. “You don’t get the attention you deserve!”
Was Mary surprised when he announced he’d be living as a beast? “Oh, no,” she says. “None of it was alien to what he’s ever been. I mean, you were living as an urban fox in London before I met you. Skulking around in the park at night.”
“I wasn’t sleeping in parks back then,” Foster clarifies. “I was trying to get into the head of an urban fox, eating out of dustbins and sometimes sleeping under bushes. I was doing that on and off most of the time I lived in London.”
Mary smiles. “I domesticated you, didn’t I?”
She says he has always been an extremist. “You suck the marrow out of it and agonise over it, to feel you’ve got to grips with it.” What form does his agonising take? “Verbal and physical. Always obsessing over the meaning of life, and what it means to be fully human. Whereas I just think, well, we’ve got to feed the kids.”
In the end, Foster’s book is a beautiful and bonkers metaphysical quest, with a number of questions at the heart of it – some universal (what does it meant to be human?), but most tied up with his own identity (what does it mean to be Charles Foster?). “All of us, I guess, wonder how real our relationships are,” he explains. “We wonder whether we are talking at cross-purposes, whether we can know anything about our nearest and dearest – this was just another lens through which to view that question. I thought, if I can have a relationship with something that is as different from me as a fox or a badger, then there’s a possibility that I might be able to know my wife or my children or my best friend.”
The revelation occurred when he was living as a fox, and came face to face with another fox that had stolen his chicken leg. “I felt not just that I was looking and observing, but that I was being looked at and being observed. That was the reciprocity I had longed for. I don’t feel I got that anywhere else.” And he came off second best to the real fox? “Oh yes! It was a better Londoner, a better liver. It could run faster, it needed less sleep, its teeth were sharper, it nose and ears were better. It was just superior.”
It made the whole exercise worthwhile, he says. “I’m reassured that relationships aren’t impossible. I’m reassured that I know something about Mary. I’m reassured that there is some core identity called Charles Foster, who’s capable of making real choices.” The angst-ridden uberman relaxes into a smile. “It’s made me slightly less insecure.”
Going Underground : meet the man who lived like an animal. By Simon Hattenstone. The Guardian, January 23, 2016
No comments:
Post a Comment